Reasoning Strategy

14,000,000 Leading Edge Experts on the ideXlab platform

Scan Science and Technology

Contact Leading Edge Experts & Companies

Scan Science and Technology

Contact Leading Edge Experts & Companies

The Experts below are selected from a list of 24792 Experts worldwide ranked by ideXlab platform

Gary D. Phye - One of the best experts on this subject based on the ideXlab platform.

  • inductive Reasoning a training approach
    Review of Educational Research, 2008
    Co-Authors: Karl Josef Klauer, Gary D. Phye
    Abstract:

    Researchers have examined inductive Reasoning to identify different cognitive processes when participants deal with inductive problems. This article presents a prescriptive theory of inductive Reasoning that identifies cognitive processing using a procedural Strategy for making comparisons. It is hypothesized that training in the use of the procedural inductive Reasoning Strategy will improve cognitive functioning in terms of (a) increased fluid intelligence performance and (b) better academic learning of classroom subject matter. The review and meta-analysis summarizes the results of 74 training experiments with nearly 3,600 children. Both hypotheses are confirmed. Further, two moderating effects were observed: Training effects on intelligence test performance increased over time, and positive problem-solving transfer to academic learning is greater than transfer to intelligence test performance. The results cannot be explained by placebo or test-coaching effects. It is concluded that the proposed strate...

Juan M Corchado - One of the best experts on this subject based on the ideXlab platform.

  • energy optimization using a case based Reasoning Strategy
    Sensors, 2018
    Co-Authors: Alfonso Gonzalezbriones, J Prieto, Fernando De La Prieta, Enrique Herreraviedma, Juan M Corchado
    Abstract:

    At present, the domotization of homes and public buildings is becoming increasingly popular. Domotization is most commonly applied to the field of energy management, since it gives the possibility of managing the consumption of the devices connected to the electric network, the way in which the users interact with these devices, as well as other external factors that influence consumption. In buildings, Heating, Ventilation and Air Conditioning (HVAC) systems have the highest consumption rates. The systems proposed so far have not succeeded in optimizing the energy consumption associated with a HVAC system because they do not monitor all the variables involved in electricity consumption. For this reason, this article presents an agent approach that benefits from the advantages provided by a Multi-Agent architecture (MAS) deployed in a Cloud environment with a wireless sensor network (WSN) in order to achieve energy savings. The agents of the MAS learn social behavior thanks to the collection of data and the use of an artificial neural network (ANN). The proposed system has been assessed in an office building achieving an average energy savings of 41% in the experimental group offices.

Steven C H Hoi - One of the best experts on this subject based on the ideXlab platform.

  • explicit memory tracker with coarse to fine Reasoning for conversational machine reading
    Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 2020
    Co-Authors: Yifan Gao, Shafiq Joty, Caiming Xiong, Richard Socher, Irwin King, Michael R Lyu, Steven C H Hoi
    Abstract:

    The goal of conversational machine reading is to answer user questions given a knowledge base text which may require asking clarification questions. Existing approaches are limited in their decision making due to struggles in extracting question-related rules and Reasoning about them. In this paper, we present a new framework of conversational machine reading that comprises a novel Explicit Memory Tracker (EMT) to track whether conditions listed in the rule text have already been satisfied to make a decision. Moreover, our framework generates clarification questions by adopting a coarse-to-fine Reasoning Strategy, utilizing sentence-level entailment scores to weight token-level distributions. On the ShARC benchmark (blind, held-out) testset, EMT achieves new state-of-the-art results of 74.6% micro-averaged decision accuracy and 49.5 BLEU4. We also show that EMT is more interpretable by visualizing the entailment-oriented Reasoning process as the conversation flows. Code and models are released at https://github.com/Yifan-Gao/explicit_memory_tracker.

  • explicit memory tracker with coarse to fine Reasoning for conversational machine reading
    arXiv: Computation and Language, 2020
    Co-Authors: Yifan Gao, Shafiq Joty, Caiming Xiong, Richard Socher, Irwin King, Michael R Lyu, Steven C H Hoi
    Abstract:

    The goal of conversational machine reading is to answer user questions given a knowledge base text which may require asking clarification questions. Existing approaches are limited in their decision making due to struggles in extracting question-related rules and Reasoning about them. In this paper, we present a new framework of conversational machine reading that comprises a novel Explicit Memory Tracker (EMT) to track whether conditions listed in the rule text have already been satisfied to make a decision. Moreover, our framework generates clarification questions by adopting a coarse-to-fine Reasoning Strategy, utilizing sentence-level entailment scores to weight token-level distributions. On the ShARC benchmark (blind, held-out) testset, EMT achieves new state-of-the-art results of 74.6% micro-averaged decision accuracy and 49.5 BLEU4. We also show that EMT is more interpretable by visualizing the entailment-oriented Reasoning process as the conversation flows. Code and models are released at this https URL.

Karl Josef Klauer - One of the best experts on this subject based on the ideXlab platform.

  • inductive Reasoning a training approach
    Review of Educational Research, 2008
    Co-Authors: Karl Josef Klauer, Gary D. Phye
    Abstract:

    Researchers have examined inductive Reasoning to identify different cognitive processes when participants deal with inductive problems. This article presents a prescriptive theory of inductive Reasoning that identifies cognitive processing using a procedural Strategy for making comparisons. It is hypothesized that training in the use of the procedural inductive Reasoning Strategy will improve cognitive functioning in terms of (a) increased fluid intelligence performance and (b) better academic learning of classroom subject matter. The review and meta-analysis summarizes the results of 74 training experiments with nearly 3,600 children. Both hypotheses are confirmed. Further, two moderating effects were observed: Training effects on intelligence test performance increased over time, and positive problem-solving transfer to academic learning is greater than transfer to intelligence test performance. The results cannot be explained by placebo or test-coaching effects. It is concluded that the proposed strate...

Oshin Vartanian - One of the best experts on this subject based on the ideXlab platform.

  • the prospects of working memory training for improving deductive Reasoning
    Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2015
    Co-Authors: Erin L Beatty, Oshin Vartanian
    Abstract:

    Cognitive (brain) training has been a major focus of study in recent years. In applied settings, the excitement regarding this research programme emanates from its prospects for far transfer—defined as observing performance benefits in outcome measures that are contextually, structurally or superficially dissimilar to the trained task (Perkins and Salomon, 1994). By and large, researchers have focused on training working memory (WM). This is not surprising, given the ubiquity of WM requirements for thinking (Baddeley, 2003). Currently, much evidence suggests that adaptive training on WM tasks can increase WM skills. In contrast, consistent evidence regarding far transfer is lacking (see Melby-Lervag and Hulme, 2013), although there is evidence to suggest that when the training modality is visuospatial, the likelihood of transfer and the long-term stability of its benefits are enhanced (Melby-Lervag and Hulme, 2013; Stephenson and Halpern, 2013). Theoretically, there is reason to suspect that interventions that increase WM skills and/or capacity could improve deductive Reasoning. This prediction stems from the observation that individual differences in WM capacity predict deductive Reasoning performance on conflict problems where the believability of conclusions conflicts with logical validity (e.g., Newstead et al., 2004). Conflict problems require WM resources because their correct solution depends on the suppression of the heuristic system (System I) in favor of responding in accordance with the analytic system (System II). Evidence for this interpretation was provided by De Neys (2006), who presented participants with conflict and non-conflict syllogisms while also burdening their executive resources with a secondary task. Specifically, the between-subjects manipulation of WM load consisted of presenting a 3 × 3 matrix prior to each syllogism, wherein the matrix was filled with a complex four-dot pattern (high load) or with three dots on a horizontal line (low load)1. After making a validity judgment, participants reproduced the matrix pattern. This experimental design required them to maintain the matrix pattern in WM while Reasoning. Whereas the high load condition impaired performance on conflict problems, there was no effect of load on non-conflict problems. This demonstrates that overcoming belief-logic conflict is limited by WM capacity. WM training could also lead to improvement in deductive Reasoning via its effect on fluid intelligence—typically measured using matrix Reasoning tasks. Specifically, much evidence suggests that general cognitive ability and deductive Reasoning are positively correlated (Stanovich and West, 2000). In addition, a recent meta-analysis demonstrated that training specifically on the n-back family of WM tasks leads to a small but positive effect on fluid intelligence (Au et al., 2014). Therefore, theoretically, increases in fluid intelligence could mediate the link between n-back training and deductive Reasoning, offering an indirect route for improving the latter (Figure ​(Figure11). Figure 1 Two possible routes for improving deductive Reasoning by working memory training. The solid arrow depicts a direct effect. The dashed arrows depict an indirect effect. Recently, Aries et al. (2014) investigated the combined effect of Reasoning Strategy and WM training on school performance. The participants for Experiment 1 were enrolled in lower-level Higher Secondary Education history classes. During the 6-week intervention period, participants in the control condition were taught using a “conservative” method that involved the introduction of new subjects in new paragraphs, and the answering of Reasoning questions from the textbook. In contrast, for participants in the experimental condition the same material was embedded within two WM training tasks: n-back and the Odd One Out. This approach ensured that training was contextualized within the subject matter of the history class. For example, on each trial of the Odd One Out four historical words or pictures were presented successively on the screen, three of which were related (e.g., were drawn from agrarian civilizations) whereas the fourth was not (i.e., was a depiction of hunter-gatherer civilization). The participant had to maintain all four stimuli in WM to select the odd one out. In the n-back task, nouns (e.g., farming) and pictures (e.g., hieroglyphics) drawn from the content of the history class were used as stimuli. In addition, the experimenters trained Reasoning strategies using a modification of the IMPROVE method (see Mevarech and Kramarski, 2003). This intervention is designed to teach the structure of Reasoning, and works by testing understanding of the problems, highlighting similarities between problems, applying strategies for solving problems, and prompting reflection on the Reasoning process. Compared to the control condition, students in the experimental condition exhibited significant gains in performance on Reasoning questions in official school tests that necessitate inference making—a difference that remained significant 16 weeks after the termination of training. Subsequently, participants in Experiment 2 who were enrolled in higher-level Higher Secondary Education history classes received either WM or Reasoning Strategy training. On its own, Reasoning Strategy but not WM training improved school test performance. The results of Aries et al. (2014) suggest that for students of relatively lower ability, the combination of WM and Reasoning Strategy training can be a successful recipe for improving Reasoning. This is likely because whereas the former enhances WM skills, the latter facilitates the acquisition of the cognitive tools for logic. For students of higher ability there might be less room for improving WM (i.e., a ceiling effect), such that learning the structure of Reasoning becomes a relatively more important factor for improving performance. Although the results of the two experiments are not directly comparable because of differences in the composition of the samples and intervention strategies, they do suggest that differences in baseline ability must be taken into account while assessing transfer effects (see Jaeggi et al., 2014). In conclusion, it appears useful to pursue the possibility that WM training could benefit deductive Reasoning directly by increasing WM skills, or indirectly by increasing fluid intelligence. Critically, Aries et al.'s successful intervention consisted of embedding WM training with domain-relevant material. It has yet to be demonstrated whether a domain-general intervention to train WM will exhibit a similar transfer profile in the context of deductive Reasoning. In addition, the extent to which successful transfer to deductive Reasoning will require supplementing WM training with Strategy training remains an open question.